The H I Mind Theory of Conscious Story-Telling.

John Cochrane – 13 May 2025

Abstract

This paper presents the second of four independent yet complementary theories within the H I Mind Model of consciousness. While each theory addresses a distinct dimension of human conscious experience, together they contribute to a more holistic understanding of the architecture and function of consciousness. The present theory explores the mechanisms by which Core Consciousness may operate, proposing that narrative simulation, supported by working memory and language-based logic, constitutes a central organizing principle of conscious experience.

1. Introduction

Human consciousness is increasingly viewed as a dynamic process involving prediction, integration, and contextual evaluation (Clark, 2013; Friston, 2010). The H I Mind Theory of Conscious Story-Telling builds upon this view by proposing that consciousness is constructed through the integration of simulations generated by various cognitive modules, or “focuses,” within a central narrative framework. This narrative process supports decision-making, environmental adaptation, and the subjective continuity of self.

2. Simulation as a Fundamental Cognitive Process

The theory posits that each cognitive focus within the H I Mind Model operates through internally generated simulations. These simulations—mental models of the world, the self, and possible actions—are based on processed sensory input and existing internal representations. The notion of simulation as a core function is supported by work in embodied cognition and predictive processing (Barsalou, 2009; Clark, 2016), which view perception, thought, and motor planning as predictive, model-driven activities.
The subconscious—or non-conscious—systems receive sensory input and generate a context-sensitive representation of the body and environment. Each conscious focus (e.g., Social, Cultural, Aspirational, Noble) interprets this representation within its domain of relevance, identifying opportunities, threats, or uncertainties.
Core Consciousness integrates these domain-specific simulations into a unified, moment-to-moment experiential model that includes real-time predictions of possible actions and outcomes. These include energetic costs, social implications, and potential rewards or dangers—functions consistent with the role of the brain's default mode network (DMN) and frontoparietal control network in mental time travel and decision forecasting (Schacter et al., 2007; Spreng et al., 2010).

3. The Role of Working Memory in Conscious Simulation

A central function of Core Consciousness is to arbitrate among competing simulations and select an optimal behavioral trajectory. This executive function is made possible by working memory—a limited-capacity system involved in holding and manipulating information over short periods (Baddeley, 2012).
Working memory enables Core Consciousness to maintain multiple simulated futures, compare projected outcomes, and adjust plans in real time. Neuroimaging evidence indicates that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and parietal regions play a critical role in these functions, supporting both cognitive control and simulation integration (Cowan, 2010; Christophel et al., 2017).

4. Language-Based Logic and Conscious Reasoning

The variability in the content and vividness of conscious awareness—ranging from sensory-rich perception to internal verbal commentary—suggests that language plays a vital role in conscious regulation. The H I Mind Theory posits that logical reasoning, planning, and introspective monitoring are supported by language-based mechanisms, primarily involving the Cultural and Noble Focuses.
This view is consistent with Vygotskian and neo-Vygotskian frameworks, which posit that internal speech mediates complex thought (Vygotsky, 1986), as well as with empirical findings showing the involvement of left-hemisphere language regions in verbal working memory and reasoning (Fedorenko and Thompson-Schill, 2014).
Language allows Core Consciousness to apply symbolic logic to simulations, abstracting key features, generalizing rules, and constructing causally coherent plans. This cognitive capacity is essential not only for problem-solving but also for self-awareness and metacognition (Carruthers, 2009).

5. Story-Telling as the Mechanism of Conscious Integration

Bringing these processes together, the H I Mind Theory proposes that Core Consciousness constructs a coherent, ongoing narrative—an internal story—that integrates multisource simulations, working memory contents, and language-based reasoning. This narrative enables individuals to track causal and temporal relations between events, actions, and motivations over time, providing the subjective sense of self-continuity and intentionality.
Narrative construction as a core function of consciousness has been proposed by a range of theorists (Bruner, 1990; Dennett, 1991), and is supported by neuroscientific research showing that episodic memory, future planning, and storytelling share common neural substrates (Hassabis and Maguire, 2007).
In this view, conscious experience is not merely a passive stream of sensory impressions but an active, story-driven synthesis of possibilities, intentions, and meaning. Consciousness, then, is best understood as a storytelling engine—a simulation-based, language-enabled, temporally structured system for making sense of ourselves and the world.

6. Conclusion

The H I Mind Theory of Conscious Story-Telling advances the hypothesis that narrative simulation is the primary cognitive structure used by Core Consciousness to manage and coordinate the operations of various domain-specific focuses. Through the integration of predictive simulation, working memory, and language-based reasoning, human consciousness is able to generate adaptive, flexible behavior and a coherent sense of self. The theory offers a framework that is consistent with current models of predictive processing, cognitive control, and narrative identity, and it invites further empirical exploration of the mechanisms linking simulation, language, and consciousness.

References

• Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29.
• Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364(1521), 1281–1289.
• Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of Meaning. Harvard University Press.
• Carruthers, P. (2009). How we know our own minds: The relationship between mindreading and metacognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(2), 121–138.
• Christophel, T. B., Klink, P. C., Spitzer, B., Roelfsema, P. R., and Haynes, J. D. (2017). The distributed nature of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(2), 111–124.
• Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181–204.
• Clark, A. (2016). Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action, and the Embodied Mind. Oxford University Press.
• Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 51–57.
• Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness Explained. Little, Brown.
• Fedorenko, E., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). Reworking the language network. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3), 120–126.
• Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127–138.
• Hassabis, D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 299–306.
• Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(9), 657–661.
• Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., and Kim, A. S. (2010). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489–510.
• Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and Language (A. Kozulin, Trans.). MIT Press.